代写范文

留学资讯

写作技巧

论文代写专题

服务承诺

资金托管
原创保证
实力保障
24小时客服
使命必达

51Due提供Essay,Paper,Report,Assignment等学科作业的代写与辅导,同时涵盖Personal Statement,转学申请等留学文书代写。

51Due将让你达成学业目标
51Due将让你达成学业目标
51Due将让你达成学业目标
51Due将让你达成学业目标

私人订制你的未来职场 世界名企,高端行业岗位等 在新的起点上实现更高水平的发展

积累工作经验
多元化文化交流
专业实操技能
建立人际资源圈

Neural development and sensorimotor control--论文代写范文精选

2016-01-16 来源: 51due教员组 类别: Essay范文

51Due论文代写网精选essay代写范文:“Neural development and sensorimotor control ” 神经系统和人的行为之间是什么关系,这是儿童心理学家和神经学家几十年来一直感兴趣的问题。这篇医学essay代写范文重新审视这个问题。近期婴儿获得的经验证据和孩子学会如何适应环境,将讨论关于潜在的神经系统的发展。然而多感官信息的整合能力和命令,要求严格的神经可塑性,可能用于自适应机器人的设计。

婴儿出生时,从控制的角度来看,两个过程的完成需要执行,任何神经控制必须能够与它进行交互。第二,视觉上指定目标必须有适当的运动行为。反过来,这些动作行为必须适合手臂移动到所需的目标。下面的essay代写范文进行详述。

Abstract 
What is the relationship between development of the nervous system and the emergence of voluntary motor behavior? This is the central question of the naturenurture discussion that has intrigued child psychologists and pediatric neurologists for decades. This paper attempts to revisit this issue. Recent empirical evidence on how infants acquire multi-joint coordination and how children learn to adapt to novel force environments will be discussed with reference to the underlying development of the nervous system. The claim will be made that the developing human nervous system by no means constitutes an ideal controller. However, its redundancy, its ability to integrate multi-modal sensory information and motor commands and its facility of time-critical neural plasticity are features that may prove to be useful for the design of adaptive robots.

Development of coordination 
At birth, a human infant can neither reach or grasp. From a control point of view, the completion of two processes are required to perform successful reaching. First, any neural controller must be capable to interact with its “plant” (i.e., the arm in this example) in such a way that centrally planned, complex actions can be executed. Second, visually specified goals must be linked to appropriate motor acts. These motor acts, in turn, must be suitable to move the arm to the desired goal. There are a number of reasons that seem to explain why newborn infants are not equipped to solve these two tasks: 
They have limited postural control of the trunk, head and arms. Appropriate head and trunk righting reactions begin to emerge 2-3 month after birth (Milani-Comparetti and Gidoni 1967). & They have limited knowledge about the physical makeup of their bodies (i.e. moments of inertia, viscosity, stiffness of their arm segments).
They have only a limited movement repertoire consisting of an array of infant reflexes (i.e., grasping, sucking), and basal intra- and interlimb synergies (coupled flexor, extensor activity, coactivation) (Bekoff et al. 1989; Hadders-Algra et al. 1992). 
They have limited visual capabilities. During the 1st postnatal month, the visual system provides the infant with functionally useful, but unrefined vision at a level of approximate 5% of adult acuity level. The infant can likely differentiate facial features from a distance of about 50 cm. Objects beyond this distance are probably not seen clearly (Atkinson and Braddick 1981). 
They have not established a finite neural control structure. Most cortico-spinal projections are not differentiated. In a first stage, cortical neurons from all areas of the neocortex send collaterals to subcortical structures - a process termed arborization. In a second stage, these collaterals are pruned according to their later function (e.g., a visual projection, or motor projection - for a review: O'Leary 1992).

Adaptive learning of dynamics 
After the onset of reaching, infants begin to manipulate objects that have quite different inertial properties. Thus, it is evident that their motor systems have the ability to adjust their motor output to changes in external forces. When adult humans perform goal-directed arm movements under the influence of an unknown external force, they learn to adapt to these external dynamics. After removal of such an external force field, they reveal kinematic after-effects that are indicative of a neural controller that still compensates the no longer existing force. Such behavior suggests that the adult human nervous system uses a neural representation of the inverse arm dynamics to control upper extremity motion. Central to the notion of an inverse dynamics model (IDM) is that learning generalizes to untrained portions of the workspace. 

Children as young as 6 years reveal such generalized adaptive learning. Learning to compensate an external damping during force elbow flexion movements transferred to the opposite hemi-field, which indicates that a model of the limb dynamics rather than an association of visited space and experienced force was acquired (Jansen-Osmann et al. 2002). Interestingly, the aftereffects were usually more pronounced in the younger children (6 yrs. vs. 10 yrs. of age), indicating that their estimations of the new force field were biased. The children also took longer to re-adapt to a normal force field, which implies that the neural representations of their actual arm dynamics in middle childhood are still not as solidified as in adults. That is, even at an age, where children routinely and successfully engage in goal-directed activities, the development of their motor control systems is not completed - the neural representations of limb dynamics still lack precision and stability.

Neural development and behavior 
It is widely established that the emergence of voluntary motor behavior does not simply constitute the unfolding of a neural plan, but that orderly neural development is also dependent on the organism’s interaction with the environment. The dependency of sensory inputs for the establishment of a functional neuronal circuitry varies between species. Lower organisms and animals with a simple motor repertoire (e.g., gait of horses) often reveal adult-like motor patterns soon after birth. In contrast, non-human primates and humans require sensory stimulation to trigger processes of neural development that will then affect the development of motor control. In addition, the plasticity of the nervous system as well as the development of efferent and afferent projections is timecritical. 

That is, the organism undergoes “critical” periods of development, where the nervous system expects certain sensory inputs. The deprivation of such stimuli prior to a critical period might have little or no detrimental effects on certain aspects of sensorimotor development, but the failure of such stimulation during such period will negatively affect later sensorimotor function. For example, rearing a monkey for its first three postnatal months under far red illumination to make color vision impossible does not result in long lasting deficits in color vision (Brenner et al. 1990). Or, Hopi Indian infants, whose mobility is restricted in the first few months through the use of cradle boards (Dennis & Dennis 1940) do not show a delayed onset of walking (Harriman & Lukosius 1982). 

In contrast, it is known that monocular deprivation of kittens during the second postnatal month leads to a striking change in the physiological organization of the visual cortex such that few cortical neurons remains responsive to the stimulation of the deprived eye (Berardi et al. 2003, Hubel & Wiesel 1964). Moreover, peripheral sensory deprivation also has a negative impact on the development of the motorneurons in mammals (McLennan & Hendry 1981), that is, the paucity of sensory signals has a direct impact on voluntary motor control. The behavioral manifestations are seen in northern Chines babies, who are reared in sand bags (for sanitary purposes), which leads to a restriction of infant’s motility. If this practice is continued beyond the first year of life, the child will not only show a delayed motor development, but it will critically impair later motor and cognitive function (Mei 1994).

Summary 
I have briefly outlined three areas of research that address the issue of how a complex system like a human child acquires motor skills, how skill learning requires adaptive force control and how neural development driven by endogenous and exogenous factors facilitates the emergence of voluntary motor control. Although the notion of brain plasticity is likely not easily implemented in an artificial system, an understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying adaptive learning and control in human infants may provide fruitful ideas for the design of adaptive robots.(essay代写)

51Due网站原创范文除特殊说明外一切图文著作权归51Due所有;未经51Due官方授权谢绝任何用途转载或刊发于媒体。如发生侵犯著作权现象,51Due保留一切法律追诉权。(essay代写)
更多essay代写范文欢迎访问我们主页 www.51due.com 当然有essay代写需求可以和我们24小时在线客服 QQ:800020041 联系交流。-X(essay代写)

上一篇:Essential properties of langua 下一篇:Cognitive Basis of Material En